These studies provide

clear evidence that the critical pe

These studies provide

clear evidence that the critical period, regardless of what triggers its onset, stays open for a limited duration of approximately 2 weeks. It is unclear what changes in the activity, biochemistry, or structure of the V1 circuit renders it no longer as susceptible to MD. Progress will depend on an understanding of how V1 is different at the end of the critical period than at its beginning, even with normal visual experience. For example, now that we understand that binocular matching of orientation selectivity progresses during the critical period (Wang et al., 2010), it appears possible that the attainment of binocular matching itself could prevent further effects of MD. After the critical period, when inputs from the two eyes produce the same pattern of responses http://www.selleckchem.com/btk.html in V1 neurons, activity through the open eye may sustain the connections serving both eyes during

MD. In contrast, before or during the critical period, when inputs from the two eyes to a particular V1 neuron are driven by different stimuli rather than coherently, they may compete, and the deprived eye would lose out. The use of mice JAK phosphorylation as a model system allowed the development of methods to measure visual responses to the two eyes in V1 repeatedly in individual animals. Transcranial optical imaging of intrinsic signals (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1996) and chronic implantation of recording electrodes to measure the amplitude of visually evoked medroxyprogesterone potentials (VEPs) both allow repeated sampling of the same brain region before, during, and after manipulations of visual experience (Kaneko et al., 2008a and Sawtell et al., 2003). Both allow reproducible measures of the magnitudes of the separate deprived- and nondeprived-eye responses. Optical imaging through an intact skull has the advantage of being noninvasive, but it is done in anesthesized animals (Kaneko et al., 2008a). VEPs have the advantage that they are commonly done in awake mice, but require precise and stable electrode placement (Sawtell et al., 2003) and

the amplitude of VEPs are susceptible to change with repeated presentations of grating stimuli of a single orientation (Frenkel et al., 2006). An alternative approach can use VEPs to measure absolute visual acuity (Fagiolini et al., 1994). Neither optical imaging nor VEPs measure the selective responses of single neurons directly. The methods above were used to dissect ODP induced by MD during the critical period into temporally distinct stages (Figure 5). In the first stage, 2–3 days of MD caused a large reduction of the response to the deprived eye and a resulting shift in ocular dominance, with no change in open-eye responses. In the second stage, MD caused a large increase in the response to the open eye, along with a slight increase in deprived-eye responses, completing the shift in ocular dominance (Kaneko et al.

To further test the idea that exposure to natural sounds influenc

To further test the idea that exposure to natural sounds influences the maturation of songbird auditory circuits, songbirds can be reared and tutored with either conspecific or heterospecific song. The intent of this manipulation is to alter the acoustics rather than the amount of auditory experience. One

study has shown that the responses of single central auditory neurons display higher information coding capacities and firing rates in male birds tutored Tanespimycin clinical trial with conspecific song, as compared to birds tutored with heterospecific song, demonstrating that experience and vocal learning are linked with auditory development (Figure 8; Woolley et al., 2010). The influence of this kind of experience on development of perceptual skills such as discriminating among the unique songs of individual birds is still unknown. The maturation of

learning, itself, provides another unique opportunity to assess auditory coding properties. As discussed above, auditory training commonly improves adult performance, but perceptual learning is poor in young animals (Sarro and Sanes, 2010 and Huyck and Wright, 2011). Although the effects of learning have yet to be assessed in the developing auditory CNS, a study in finches has shown that, at the level of sound production, forebrain motor neurons display increased temporal precision and rate as finches practice their songs (Crandall et al., 2007). Even before hair cells are activated by sound, they discharge spontaneously and release transmitter, thereby eliciting Selleckchem Perifosine bursts of action potentials in primary auditory neurons, which then excites central circuits (Tritsch et al., 2010 and Johnson et al., 2011). The presence of spontaneous activity prior to sensory activation has been implicated in the maturation of synaptic connections in the visual system (Feller, 2009) and may serve a similar role in auditory development. For example, inhibitory projections Sclareol from the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) to the lateral superior olive (LSO) undergo a dramatic period of functional refinement before the onset of hearing (Kim and Kandler, 2003). During this

period, MNTB afferents release three transmitters (GABA, glycine, glutamate), and disruption of glutamate release prevents functional refinement (Gillespie et al., 2005 and Noh et al., 2010). A current model suggests that the prehearing period of spontaneous activity-dependent synapse maturation may lead to a second phase during which silenced synapses are anatomically eliminated (Kandler et al., 2009). Thus, activity-dependent plasticity occurs well in advance of acoustic experience. Our emphasis on the prolonged time course of perceptual maturation implies that certain synaptic or biophysical properties must also be late developing. However, studies that chart the maturation of these cellular properties generally report that maturation occurs rapidly, usually within 7–14 days of hearing onset, in rodents.

In line with this hypothesis, we have previously reported—using t

In line with this hypothesis, we have previously reported—using the same deprivation paradigm—that the turnover of spines, which is typically associated with synapse specific (or nonhomeostatic) plasticity (Trachtenberg et al., 2002, Zuo et al., 2005 and Holtmaat et al., 2006), increases 72 hr after deprivation (Keck et al., 2008).

Taking these data together, we suggest the following scenario: immediately after a complete lesion of both retinae, cortical activity decreases to approximately half the original value. Synaptic scaling then manifests itself after 24 hr, at which time mEPSC amplitudes and spine sizes have increased, followed by a decrease in inhibition after 48 hr. The overall increase

in synaptic strength, together with a reduction in inhibition, leads to a nearly complete restoration of cortical activity levels; however, selleck chemical feedforward inputs NVP-BKM120 order are not restored. Thus, after 72 hr, dendritic spine turnover increases (Keck et al., 2008), potentially reflecting the search for novel active inputs and further circuit rearrangement. An extended description of the experimental procedures is included in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures. All experimental procedures carried out at the Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology were performed in accordance with the institutional guidelines of the Max Planck Society and the local government DNA ligase (Regierung

von Oberbayern). All experimental procedures carried out at the Friedrich Miescher Institute in Basel were approved by the Veterinary Department of the Canton of Basel-Stadt, Switzerland. Complete retinal lesions were carried out as described previously (Keck et al., 2008). At 6, 18, 24, or 48 hr after the retinal/sham lesion, coronal slices were prepared from C57BL/6J mice. Visualized whole-cell patch-clamp recordings of layer 5 pyramidal neurons were performed at room temperature (24°C). mEPSCs or mIPSCs were recorded in voltage clamp at −70 mV (corrected for liquid junction potential). mEPSC and mIPSC analysis was done with custom software, blind to the experimental condition. Events were detected based on amplitudes greater than 5 pA and 20%–80% rise times of less than 1 ms (Desai et al., 2002). Experiments were carried out as described previously (Keck et al., 2008). Cranial windows were implanted (Holtmaat et al., 2009) in adult mice expressing enhanced GFP (eGFP) under the thy-1 promoter (GFP-M line [ Feng et al., 2000]). The visual cortex was localized using intrinsic signal imaging prior to retinal lesions. Apical dendrites in layer 1 and 2/3 (0–150 μm below the pial surface) of layer 5 cells in monocular visual cortex were imaged using a custom-built two-photon laser-scanning microscope.

These data indicate that basic synaptic function matures normally

These data indicate that basic synaptic function matures normally but elimination of redundant CFs is impaired in GAD67+/GFP mice. We then investigated innervation pattern of CFs morphologically by anterograde labeling of CFs with dextran Texas red (DTR) (Figures 2B–2H, red) combined with immunofluorescence for a PC marker, calbindin (Figures 2B–2H, blue or ocher), and a CF terminal

marker, vesicular glutamate transporter type 2 (VGluT2) Sirolimus solubility dmso (Figures 2B–2H, green). In both mice, DTR-labeled CFs precisely followed the PC’s proximal dendrites and climbed up to the four-fifths of the molecular layer (Figures 2B and 2E). In control mice, terminals of DTR-labeled CFs were completely overlapped with VGluT2 immunoreactivity throughout dendritic arbors of each PC, indicating predominant mono-innervation patterns. At PC somata of control mice, VGluT2-positive terminals were rarely observed (Figures 2C and 2D), reflecting dendritic translocation of CFs during development. In contrast, PC somata of GAD67+/GFP mice were often associated with DTR-labeled/VGluT2-positive terminals

and DTR-unlabeled/VGluT2-positive terminals (red and green arrows in Figures 2F1 and 2G1, respectively). In some cases, proximal shaft dendrites were innervated by the two types of CF terminals (red and green selleck arrows in Figure 2H1). These results indicate that reduction of GAD67 leads to incomplete pruning of surplus CFs, resulting in multiple innervation of PCs by CFs. To examine at which stage of postnatal development the impairment occurs in GAD67+/GFP mice, we followed developmental course of CF innervation from P5 to P20. At P5–P6, just before the onset of CF synapse elimination, all PCs were innervated by four or more CFs in control and GAD67+/GFP mice (Figure 3A) with no significant difference in the frequency distribution of PCs as to the number of CF-EPSC steps (p = 0.635; Figure 3A). At P7–P9, the frequency distribution histograms of PCs were significantly shifted toward smaller numbers from those at

P5–P6, but no statistical significance was observed between the two mouse strains (p = 0.292; Figure 3B). At P10–P12, while nearly 70% of PCs were innervated by one or two CFs in control PDK4 mice, 60% of PCs remained innervated by more than three CFs in GAD67+/GFP mice. Control PCs were innervated by significantly fewer CFs than GAD67+/GFP PCs (p = 0.006; Figure 3C). At P13–P15 and P16–P20, the difference became even larger (p < 0.001). The proportion of PCs innervated by single CFs increased to about 70% in control mice, whereas nearly 70% of PCs remained innervated by multiple CFs in GAD67+/GFP mice (Figures 3D and 3E). To test whether functional differentiation into “strong” and “weak” CFs proceeds normally in GAD67+/GFP mice, we calculated the disparity index and disparity ratio (Hashimoto and Kano, 2003).

The field has identified

that adult neurogenesis occurs i

The field has identified

that adult neurogenesis occurs in at least these two regions in rodents through nonhuman primates (e.g., Imayoshi et al., 2008 and Kornack and Rakic, 2001) and in human dentate gyrus as assessed directly using BrdU in cancer patients (Eriksson et al., 1998). Adult neurogenesis in the OB and dentate gyrus has been increasingly implicated in, and demonstrated to function in, olfactory and spatial learning and memory, respectively. Connections to learning and memory make these processes especially interesting, for at least two distinct sets of reasons. Neratinib nmr First, because of the core puzzle of how brain circuitry modifies itself with learning—at the levels of molecular changes, synaptic spine changes, connectivity changes, and even via insertion of new neurons by adult neurogenesis. The second is that adult neurogenesis, and reductions thereof, have been implicated in many human disease states (with varying levels of supporting data and plausibility), from major affective psychiatric disease, to neurodegenerative diseases like Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, selleck products to drug abuse and addiction. Thus, adult neurogenesis, and by its central place in that field, adult

OB neurogenesis, have assumed positions that are seen to touch upon much broader issues of learning, memory, cognition, plasticity, disease, regeneration, and—yes—even the question of our uniqueness as humans with regard to mental complexity and function. There has been a relatively recent controversy about whether all the deeply interesting results in the field regarding OB neurogenesis in rodents are even relevant in humans. Does the rostral migratory stream (RMS) through which newborn OB neurons migrate in rodents Isotretinoin through nonhuman primates even exist in humans? Is there evidence of continued neuroblast migration through an RMS in postmortem

human brains? Does that reduce to a trickle or less in adult humans? There is compelling evidence that this system is smaller, different in form, and substantially reduced after infancy (Sanai et al., 2004 and Sanai et al., 2011), but work by others indicates that, though its anatomy is altered by brain expansion, a functional RMS exists (Curtis et al., 2007 and Wang et al., 2011). Other work identifies some progenitors directly within the OB itself, perhaps an additional local source for human adult OB neurogenesis (Pagano et al., 2000). Taken together, the system in humans appears different to some or great extent, but is it unique? Does it function at all? In this issue of Neuron, Bergmann et al. (2012) report that adult human OB neurogenesis with long-term neuronal survival is extremely limited … at least in a limited cohort of Swedes, many of whom with neuropsychiatric disease and substance abuse. The authors apply state-of-the-art approaches of 14C cell birth dating that their labs developed several years ago ( Spalding et al.

There is an additional population of neurons in the supramammilla

There is an additional population of neurons in the supramammillary region and extending laterally to selleck products the subthalamic nucleus, which is

a known source of projections to the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain (Grove, 1988 and Saper, 1985). Many neurons in this region express the vesicular glutamate transporter 2 (Hur and Zaborszky, 2005 and Ziegler et al., 2002) but whether these glutamatergic neurons promote arousal remains to be determined. The most rostral population of arousal-promoting subcortical neurons is located in the basal forebrain. Many of these neurons contain either acetylcholine or gamma-amino-butyric acid (GABA), and a small number contain glutamate ( Manns et al., 2001 and Hur and Zaborszky, 2005). Basal forebrain cholinergic neurons innervate, both directly and indirectly activate cortical Bcl-2 inhibitor pyramidal cells, and probably augment cortical activation and EEG desynchronization ( Jones, 2004). GABAergic basal forebrain neurons innervate and presumably inhibit cortical GABAergic interneurons and deep layer pyramidal cells ( Freund and Meskenaite, 1992 and Henny and Jones, 2008), both of which most likely result in disinhibition of cortical circuits.

Many of these basal forebrain neurons are wake-active and fire in bursts correlated with specific EEG rhythms. Small ibotenic acid lesions of the basal forebrain result in modest slowing of the EEG without changing the amount of wake or sleep, while specific lesions of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons reduce wakefulness transiently, without affecting the EEG frequency spectrum ( Kaur et al., 2008). On the other hand, acute inactivation Calpain of the basal forebrain with the anesthetic procaine produces deep NREM sleep, whereas activation with glutamatergic agonists causes wakefulness ( Cape and Jones, 2000). A definitive understanding of the roles of the basal forebrain cell groups in arousal awaits studies that differentially eliminate the GABAergic population. The thalamic relay nuclei (such

as the anterior, ventral, and lateral thalamic cell groups; medial and lateral geniculate nuclei; mediodorsal nucleus; and pulvinar) are the most important and abundant sources of subcortical glutamatergic afferents to the cerebral cortex, and the intralaminar and midline nuclei provide a diffuse source of cortical input ( Jones and Leavitt, 1974). Surprisingly, there is little evidence that these inputs play a major role in producing wakefulness. Early electrical stimulation studies suggested that the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei might constitute a diffuse, nonspecific cortical activating system ( Morison and Dempsey, 1942 and Steriade, 1995), but lesions of the midline and intralaminar nuclei did not prevent cortical activation ( Moruzzi and Magoun, 1949 and Starzl et al., 1951).

, 2006, Nobre et al , 2006, Mirabella et al , 2007 and Wegener et

, 2006, Nobre et al., 2006, Mirabella et al., 2007 and Wegener et al., 2008). In contrast to feature tagging of objects, sorting or classifying of objects on

the basis of one elemental feature dimension (e.g., color of fruit or shape of fruit, Figure 7A) requires that the unity of perceptual objects be broken down. This type of feature attention was directly examined in a study in which the activity of V4 neurons was recorded while an animal was attending to either the color or the orientation feature dimension of colored oriented bars (four possible colors and four possible orientations) ( Mirabella et al., 2007). Monkeys were trained to turn a response lever to the selleck screening library left in response to two of the four colors (red and blue) and two of the four orientations (0° and 45°), and to the right in response to the other two colors (yellow and green) and two orientations (90° and 135°). Monkeys responded (left or right) to color-orientation pairings. To perform the task correctly, the monkeys had to selectively attend to the feature dimension that was cued, while ignoring the other feature dimension. The study

reported that responses of V4 neurons to otherwise identical stimuli are modulated depending on the task cue ( Figure 7B); Dabrafenib manufacturer remarkably, the selected task-relevant features were “selected” into one of two behaviorally relevant response categories (left versus right). This type of task-dependent neuronal response grouping provides the first evidence that network associations in V4 can be directed, not only Tolmetin by sensory-defined features, but also by top-down motor output categories. Neuronal firing rate may not be the only means by which attentional signals are mediated. Recent findings suggest that feature-based attention may also act by increasing synchronization among the neurons selective for the relevant features, particularly in the gamma-band (35–70 hz) frequency range (Bichot et al.,

2005, Taylor et al., 2005 and Womelsdorf and Fries, 2007). In a visual search task that contained an array of objects defined by both color and shape, Bichot et al. (2005) showed that gamma band oscillations occurred more frequently when attended targets fell in the receptive field (both initially and prior to eye movements), suggesting a role for synchrony in feature-guided serial and parallel search. Such enhancements in gamma band oscillation have also been reported to occur during spatial attention tasks (Fries et al., 2001). Furthermore, other studies report that attentional modulation leads to decreased firing rate synchronization in V4, and proposed this as a way to reduce correlated noise and thus enhance signal-to-noise (Mitchell et al., 2009 and Cohen and Maunsell, 2009). Participation of enhanced versus decreased correlation may be cell type specific. Mitchell et al.

, 2002, Nishiki and Augustine, 2004, Shin et al , 2009 and Lee et

, 2002, Nishiki and Augustine, 2004, Shin et al., 2009 and Lee et al., 2013), whereas Ca2+ triggering of asynchronous release required the C2A domain Ca2+-binding sites of Syt7 (Figure 5). Most of our findings were Alectinib nmr supported by KD manipulations with multiple independent shRNAs, by rescue experiments with WT and mutant Syt1 and Syt7 cDNAs, and/or by KO experiments for both Syt1 and Syt7. Thus, we propose that Syt1 and Syt7 perform overall similar

functions in Ca2+ triggering of release, although with different time courses, C2 domain mechanisms, and efficiencies. Besides blocking evoked synchronous release, deletion of Syt1 greatly increases spontaneous minirelease; this increased minirelease is also Ca2+ dependent but exhibits a different apparent Ca2+ affinity and Ca2+ cooperativity than minirelease in WT neurons (Xu et al., 2009). We show that although Syt7 is required for most Ca2+-triggered asynchronous

release, the Syt7 KD did not decrease the >10-fold elevated minifrequency in Syt1 KO neurons (Figures 4 and 6C). In contrast, overexpression of WT Syt7 but not of mutant Syt7 suppressed the elevated minifrequency in Syt1 KO neurons. Even for clamping spontaneous minirelease, Syt1 and Syt7 differed in their C2 domain requirements in that the clamping activity of Syt7 required only its WT C2A domain, whereas the clamping activity of Syt1 require both its WT C2A and its WT C2B domain (Figure 5D). Our data extend previous studies on Syt1 by confirming its central role as Ca2+ sensor for fast synchronous release (Geppert et al., 1994, Fernández-Chacón out et al., 2001 and Mackler Selleck MK 2206 et al., 2002). Our results also complement earlier studies on Syt7 that documented a major role for Syt7 in neuroendocrine exocytosis (Sugita et al., 2001, Shin et al., 2002, Fukuda

et al., 2004, Tsuboi and Fukuda, 2007, Schonn et al., 2008, Gustavsson et al., 2008, Gustavsson et al., 2009, Li et al., 2009 and Segovia et al., 2010). Moreover, our findings confirm that KO of Syt7 in WT neurons produces no significant phenotype in release elicited by extracellular stimulation (Maximov et al., 2008) and agree with the observation that Syt7 supports asynchronous release during extended stimulus trains in the zebrafish neuromuscular junction (Wen et al., 2010). However, our observations conflict with our own previous finding that constitutive Syt1/Syt7 double KO mice do not exhibit an additional phenotype compared to Syt1 KO mice (Maximov et al., 2008)—indeed, this discrepancy prompted us to institute multiple levels of controls here to confirm the specificity of the observed effects. A possible explanation of this discrepancy is that our earlier experiments involved constitutive KOs that may have elicited developmental compensation. Our data also argue against a recent suggestion that Doc2A and Doc2B proteins are Ca2+ sensors for asynchronous release and that a KD of Doc2A alone impairs release in hippocampal neurons because hippocampal neurons express only Doc2A (Yao et al.

As an alternative, we hypothesized that nicotine administration a

As an alternative, we hypothesized that nicotine administration altered the DA and GABA responses to alcohol through a neuroendocrine signal (Armario, 2010). Stress-related hormones, such as glucocorticoids, cause long-term homeostatic changes in neural function and influence DA and GABA transmission (Barrot et al., 2000, Butts et al., 2011 and Joëls and Baram, 2009). Nicotine activates the HPA axis to increase plasma levels of corticosterone (Lutfy et al., 2012), the principle glucocorticoid

CHIR-99021 in vivo in rodents, which we confirmed (Figure S3). To determine whether glucocorticoid receptor activation during nicotine pretreatment contributes to subsequent alterations in ethanol-induced DA release, we systemically blocked glucocorticoid receptors with RU486 (Cadepond et al., 1997) prior to nicotine pretreatment. Pretreatment with RU486 (Figure 5A, blue circles) prevented the inhibitory effect of nicotine on ethanol-induced DA release (group × time: F(10,240) = 4.75, p < 0.01). PD173074 This increased DA response to ethanol after RU486 and nicotine pretreatment

was not distinguishable from the control rats pretreated with saline alone or RU486 alone ( Figure 5A, dashed trace). These results suggested that stress hormone receptor activation within the VTA, after nicotine pretreatment, attenuated the subsequent DA response to ethanol. To test this hypothesis, we blocked glucocorticoid receptors locally in the VTA with RU486 prior to nicotine pretreatment. The control group that received a local intra-VTA microinfusion of vehicle followed by nicotine pretreatment showed a decreased DA response to ethanol 15 hr later (Figure 5B, red circles), consistent with our previous data (see Figure 1). This inhibitory effect of nicotine pretreatment was prevented by intra-VTA microinfusion of RU486 prior to nicotine pretreatment (Figure 5B, blue circles) (group × time: F(10,140) =

2.43, p < 0.05). We should note that the intra-VTA RU486 did not completely reverse much the effect of nicotine pretreatment. A post hoc comparison indicated a significant difference between the saline control ( Figure 5B, dashed line) and the group pretreated with intra-VTA RU486 + Nic (F(10,220) = 2.01, p < 0.05). The microinfusion sites were dispersed mainly in the more ventral VTA, including the anterior and posterior regions ( Figure 5C). There was no consistent relationship between the microinfusion site and the individual DA responses to ethanol in either group. As a negative control, microinfusion of RU486 outside and adjacent to the VTA did not reverse the inhibitory effect of nicotine pretreatment (n = 3).

This cancellation of self-generated sensory

This cancellation of self-generated sensory INCB024360 clinical trial feedback would be used to increase the detection of any environmentally generated sensory information (Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001). One of the ways that this theory was tested was by using the observation that self-generated tickle was much less ticklish than externally generated tickle. By using robotic manipulanda to separate the self-generated motion to perform the tickle and the tactile input on the skin (giving rise to the tickle sensation), it was demonstrated that as the sensation was changed from the self-generated motion by adding small delays or changes in movement direction, the tactile input became

more ticklish (Blakemore et al., 1999). This demonstrates that the prediction mechanism used in sensory perception was precise, both spatially and temporally. A similar effect was found in force generation, where selleck products self-generated forces are felt less intensively. This was used

to explain the finding of force escalation (Shergill et al., 2003). Support for this idea that the efference copy is used to predict the sensory consequences of movement and remove this for sensory perception has also been found in self-generated head movements where the predicted cancellation signal is subtracted in the vestibular nuclei (Roy and Cullen, 2001 and Roy and Cullen, 2004). Research on eye movements has also provided strong evidence for the use of efference

copy in a manner that illustrates many of the properties of the forward model, in particular for this transformation from motor to sensory representation (Roy and Cullen, 2001, Roy and Cullen, 2004, Sommer and Wurtz, 2002 and Sommer and Wurtz, 2006). In the visual system, the change in afferent feedback produced by the movement of the eye needs to be determined Tryptophan synthase in order to discount accurately the self-generated movement (reafference) from the externally generated movement in the world (exafference). This could be done using the motor signals sent to the muscle of the eye. Saccades are generated from the frontal eye field (FEF) via descending drive through the superior colliculus (SC) (for a review see Andersen and Buneo, 2002); therefore, it was hypothesized that signals from the SC could act as efference copy back to the FEF (Sommer and Wurtz, 2002). One candidate pathway, therefore, was via the medial dorsal nucleus (MD) of the thalamus, which increases activity just prior to the saccade and signals the direction of the saccade (Sommer and Wurtz, 2004a) (Figure 2A). In the double-step saccade task (Figure 2B), two targets are flashed sequentially during fixation, to which the eye is then required to make a saccade to in sequence. The location of the second target is only available as a vector from the initial fixation position.